Research Article
Print
Research Article
Females of the four-eyed frog, Pleurodema thaul (Anura, Leptodactylidae), respond behaviourally to conspecific male scent
expand article infoValentina Rojas, Antonieta Labra§|, José Luis Valdés, Nelson A. Velásquez
‡ Universidad Católica del Maule, Talca, Chile
§ University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| NGO Vida Nativa, Santiago, Chile
¶ Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
Open Access

Abstract

Among amphibians, conspecific chemical communication has been widely studied in Caudata. Adult anurans, by contrast, have received less attention. Recently, it was shown that chemical scents are also relevant for adult anuran intraspecific communication. In this context, we evaluate whether females of the four-eyed frog (Pleurodema thaul) respond to conspecific male scents. We carried out a double choice experiment in a Y-maze. Females were repeatedly presented with the scents of several males versus distilled water. To extract the scent from males, we acoustically stimulated males and then used the water from their aquaria for the experiments. Our data suggest that females are capable of responding behaviourally to male scents, since they spent longer periods in the zones with male scent, rather than in zones with water. We propose that under natural breeding conditions, females of P. thaul may use either their chemical sense or chemical cues to facilitate their encounters with males.

Key Words

anuran communication, chemical communication, female choice, male scent

Introduction

Animal communication is a process by which information is transferred from a sender to a receiver, provoking in the receiver a behavioural or physiological reaction (Dusenbery 1992). This communication process is essential for social interactions (e.g. agonistic interactions, courtship rituals), and can depend on different channels (e.g. acoustic, visual, vibrational, chemical), which might be utilised independently or in association (Partan 2004; Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011).

Chemical communication is one of the oldest systems of communication in the animal kingdom, and it has been studied across ontogenetic stages and in different social situations across several taxa (Gorman and Mills 1984; Roberts et al. 2003; Wyatt 2014). Particularly in amphibians, most knowledge regarding conspecific chemical communication has been developed for Caudata, where scents have a significant role in different aspects of their social life (Park and Sung 2006; Houck et al. 2007, 2008). For instance, salamanders of the family Plethodontidae have been widely studied for the use of chemical signals in their social interactions (Guillaume 2000; Rollinson and Hackett 2015). It was observed that males of Plethodon shermani produce a sexual pheromone denominated Plethodon Receptivity Factor (PRF) that increases the female`s receptivity (Rollmann et al. 1999). In addition, courtship behaviour involving pheromone delivery (Houck 2009), the genetic components for sex pheromone productions and their evolution across amphibian groups, have been widely described (Watts et al. 2004). The genus Cynops, has received strong attention for their intersexual interactions mediated by chemosignals (Toyoda et al. 2004). Kikuyama et al. (1995), discovered that males of the fire belly salamander (C. phyrrogaster) release a female-attracting pheromone (denominated Sodefrina) and males of C. ensicauda release a peptide from their abdominal gland called Silefrin that attracted females (Yamamoto et al. 2000). The fire belly salamander and their Sodefrin pheromone have become a model for the study of sex pheromones in aquatic salamanders, resulting in the discovery of a multi-component pheromone system (Van Bocxlaer et al. 2016).

Anurans have been described as acoustically dependent; several studies have, however, by now demonstrated that scents can mediate conspecific recognition (Kiseleva 1996; Carlson et al. 2015), provide information about kinship identity (Waldman 1985) and play a role in predator-prey interactions in the larval stages of anurans (Pearl et al. 2003; Chivers and Ferrari 2013; Troyer and Turner 2015). Even learning of chemical cues at embryonic stages has been described (Hepper and Waldman 1992). Additionally, chemical signals are also important in different social contexts in adult anurans (Kam 2002; Still et al. 2019). For instance, Leiopelma hamiltoni releases a chemical compound in the faeces that provides information to conspecific individuals about the size of the sender (Lee and Waldman 2002). In courtship situations, it has been observed that males of Litoria splendida release a sexual pheromone called Splendipherin that elicits changes in postural and permanence times in females (Wabnitz 1999). A recent study has contributed knowledge about anuran chemical communication through the study of compound secretions (Brunetti et al. 2019) and also several investigations provided evidence for their role in social interactions (Pearl et al. 2000; Asay et al. 2005; King et al. 2005). For example, Byrne and Keogh (2007) showed that the Australian terrestrial Pseudophryne bibronii toadlets use scents to identify and locate potential mates. In addition, the frog Lithobates pipiens shows differences in their behavioural responses to adult male and female chemical cues (Graham et al. 2020). Compared with other amphibian groups, however, more investigation describing chemical communication in adult anurans is still needed.

Adult males of the four-eyed frog, Pleurodema thaul, emit advertisement calls during the reproductive season, inflating their vocal sacs while floating on the surface of slow-flowing streams or pools (Penna and Veloso 1990; Velásquez et al. 2014). Research has shown that females do not exhibit a preference for males based on their calls (Velásquez et al. 2015). Alternatively, their preference may be guided by male scent. Currently, the only information available on chemosensory recognition in this species focuses on tadpoles using chemicals under predation risk (Pueta et al. 2016; Pueta and Perotti 2016), but the question of whether adults utilise scent recognition has not been studied yet. In this context, and considering the available information on sexual scents involved in mate attraction in other anurans (Wabnitz et al. 1999), we aimed to determine whether female Pleurodema thaul responded behaviourally to male scents.

Methods

Subjects

We collected a total of 13 individuals (3 males and 10 females) of Pleurodema thaul at Puente Negro (34°40'S, 70°53'W) in central Chile, and transported them to the laboratory in Talca, Chile. All specimens were separated by sex and housed individually in glass aquaria (15 × 20 × 30 cm) with water and gravel. The individuals were maintained in an animal room with an inverted photoperiod (12Light: 12Dark), with a temperature of 21 ± 1 °C (mean ± SD), and were fed tenebrionid larvae once a week, according to the described care instruction for laboratory animals (National Research Council 2010). After finishing all experiments and re-adapting the frogs to their normal photoperiod, we returned all of them in a healthy condition to their geo-referenced collecting point.

Conspecific male scents solution

To obtain male scent during their reproductive state (see Chu and Wilczynski 2001), we acoustically stimulated males as follows: after one week of acclimation, each male P. thaul was isolated for 10 days in an aquarium (20 × 8 × 15 cm), containing 50 ml of distilled water and previously washed gravel. To simulate the acoustic conditions experienced by males at night during the breeding season, we placed all 3 males in their individual aquaria, into a semi-anechoic chamber (with the walls and ceiling covered with a 25 cm-high foam wedge) to avoid sound reverberation and external noises. The male advertisement call was continuously broadcasted for one hour in complete darkness. We repeated this for 10 consecutive days, according to Chu and Wilczynski (2001). For the playbacks, we used a personal computer (Macbook Unibody, Apple Inc. Cupertino, CA, USA), controlling the amplitude with a programmable attenuator (PA5, Tucker-Davis Technologies). The calls were amplified (NAD C326BEE, NAD Electronics, Pickering, Canada), and broadcasted via two loudspeakers (Behringer 1C, Behringer, Willich, Germany) with an intensity of 70 dB SPL RMS (Penna and Solís 1998; Penna et al. 2008) measured inside each aquarium with a sound level meter (Bruel and Kjaer 2238; Bruel & Kjaer Instruments, Inc., MA, USA). The aquaria were placed on an anti-vibration table and the loudspeakers were located 20 cm away from the aquaria. After stimulating the males for 10 days, we followed the methodology previously tested by Waldman and Bishop (2004) and Schulte and Rössler (2013) to extract and store water samples from each male. Immediately at the end of acoustic stimulation, we took water samples from each male’s aquarium (henceforth, conspecific scent solution), and stored them separately at 4 °C for three days, until the olfactory discrimination experiments.

Olfactory discrimination experiments

Experiments were carried out in a semi-anechoic chamber. Following Pearl et al. (2000), we tested the discrimination ability of females (N=10; mean ± SD; weight=4.32 ±1.79 g; snout-vent length= 37.16 ± 3.81 mm) using a Y-maze made out of plexiglass walls (Fig. 1). At the base of the Y-maze, in the starting zone (10 × 10 × 10 cm), we placed the focal female, one minute before the experiment. We installed an acoustic-transparent plastic mesh at the end of each arm behind which, we placed a loudspeaker. Along the floor of one arm of the maze (15 × 10 × 10 cm), we provided a scent trail using 5 ml of the conspecific scent solution, and in the other, we used 5 ml of water-control (see in Fig. 1, as yellow and blue arm, respectively). The position of each solution was interchanged throughout the experiments to avoid potential position bias.

Figure 1. 

Graphical representation of the set-up of the Y-maze experiment. The maze was divided into different zones, where yellow represents the presence of conspecific scent solution and blue represents the control solution. The box at the base of the Y-maze represents the starting zone into which females were placed before the beginning of each experiment.

While females were in the starting zone of the Y-maze, we simultaneously broadcasted a conspecific advertisement call from each loudspeaker at the end of each arm of the maze for one minute, to stimulate movement in the females. After the acoustic stimulation, we lifted a gate, and monitored the female’s reaction for five minutes (Fig. 1). We video-recorded the focal individual with a digital camera (SONY HDR-PJ760V, SONY Inc, Tokyo, Japan) placed in a zenithal position at 1 m above the centre of the maze. The experiments were carried out in darkness, only using an infrared LED bulb that was placed next to the digital camera (which had its infrared sensor activated). Experiments were monitored in real time from the outside of the semi-anechoic chamber using a computer screen connected to the digital camera. At the end of each experiment, the focal female was placed back into her aquarium and the Y-maze was washed with 70% ethanol and dried out to eliminate all chemical traces. Videos were downloaded to an Apple computer (iMac, Apple Inc. Cupertino, CA, USA) and formatted to AVI files with the VIRTUALDUB software (Copyright 1998–2009 by Avery Lee, version 1.9.7) for further analysis. The behavioural responses of the females were analysed with a custom-made MATLAB video tracking routine (Matlab 2007b, MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). From these routines, we established different zones inside the Y-maze (Fig. 1), sectioning the base of the Y-maze into three squares and each arm into two squares, establishing a triangle at the junction between the base and the arms. Finally, we tracked the position of the females continuously during each trial.

The female’s reaction to the conspecific scent solution was tested twice using the scent of each of the three males, resulting in a total of 6 experiments per female. We measured the female’s choice towards the conspecific scent solution vs. water-control, by monitoring the first choice (i.e. the arm which a female entered first). We also measured the time spent in each zone across the maze (i.e. permanence time) of the Y-maze. Female choices towards the arm dowsed with conspecific scent solution out of all trial were analysed using the Binomial tests, while the differences in time spent within the arm including the conspecific scent solution versus the arm with the water-control were analysed using the Sign test. To compare the behavioural responses between the first and second trials we used a time index: (M-C/M+C), where M is the time spent in the zones with the conspecific scent solution and C the time spent in the water control zones. The indexes of both trials were compared using U-Mann-Whitney tests. Finally, to determinate whether the male identity influenced the female responses, we performed a Kruskal-Wallis test on the time indexes obtained from the female responses to each male. All statistics analysis were performed in STATISTICA 8.0 (StatSoft. Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA).

All the protocols of this study were previously approved by the Ethics Committee of the Universidad Católica del Maule (CICUAL 2018-04 FCBUCM) and complied with regulations for the animal care and conservation in Chile (SAG, Livestock and Agriculture Service, permit N° 5641).

Results

A total of 60 trials were done, and, in 80% of these cases, females showed a positive response toward the scent of males by entering the corresponding arm. Despite this observation, females chose the arm with the conspecific male scent and the arm with the control solution indiscriminately (One-tailed Binomial test: p = 0.385, Fig. 2A), but they spent more time in the male scent solution than in the control solution arm (Sign test: Z = 2.33; p < 0.05, Figs 2B, 3). The analysis of permanence time indexes showed that females were consistent between replicates (U-Mann Whitney test: Z = -0.483; p = 0.629), and the identity of the male did not affect the response of the females (Kruskal-Wallis test: H = 2.736, p = 0.741).

Figure 2. 

Total number of choices (A), and permanence time (B) shown by females of Pleurodema thaul in the Y-maze experiment. The asterisk indicates significant differences (p < 0.05).

Figure 3. 

Heat-map showing the permanence time of females of Pleurodema thaul in the Y-maze. A) represent arm containing male scents and B) represent water-control arm. The lateral bar indicates the relation between colour and the permanence time (s). Heat-map is the outcome of overlaying track routine videos.

Discussion

Females of Pleurodema thaul exhibit a behavioural response towards the scents of conspecific males by remaining in the arm containing the conspecific scent for a longer period compared to the arm containing water, suggesting that females of P. thaul are capable of detecting chemical cues originating from conspecific males. This, however, was not reflected in the first-choice behaviour of the females. Females seemed to choose both arms at similar rates.

The lack of initial choice for the arm dowsed with a male’s scent may be explained by females depending on water-soluble compounds during mate choice. During the breeding season, male P. thaul congregate in pools to call and attract females (Penna and Veloso 1990; Velásquez et al. 2015). Females might need to be in close contact with water so as to show a behavioural response, similar to what was observed in Litoria splendida (Wabnitz et al. 2000). The water-soluble sexual pheromone Splendipherin produced by L. splendida males was effective in eliciting female attraction when the aquarium had a water column depth of 2 cm (Wabnitz et al. 2000). The lack of female choice for the conspecific scents in P. thaul, together with the longer period they spent in the arm with the conspecifics scent, suggests that females depend on water-soluble molecules to elicit specific behavioural responses, as it has been previously proposed (Wyatt 2010). Therefore, it is likely that similar experiments that include more water (e.g., Pearl et al. 2000; Asay et al. 2005) may trigger more robust behavioural responses in females of P. thaul.

Despite the lack of first choice preference, females remained for longer periods in zones containing the conspecific scent solution, hinting that females reacted to the scents produced by males. This result might indicate that chemical compounds promote intersexual interactions in P. thaul. Females may alter their locomotion or activity levels to move in the direction of the scent source in order to find a mate (Norris and Lopez 2011). Similar results were obtained in the dwarf African clawed frog (Hymenochirus sp.) in which females were attracted to water containing male scent in a two-choice aquatic Y-maze and then remained for longer periods closer to the chemical scent source, suggesting an intersexual recognition mediated by scent (Pearl et al. 2000). Permanence time has been used to determinate the existence of conspecific chemical communication across different anuran species (Lee and Waldman 2002; Graham et al. 2020). Thus, considering the time spent in male scented zones and that conspecific male solution used in this experiment could resemble pool water during a breeding season, our results are in concordance with previous studies that demonstrate how chemical communication plays an important role in conspecific interactions. A previous study showed that females of P. thaul do not exhibit a preference for males based on their calls (Velásquez et al. 2015). Potentially, females of P. thaul need both acoustic and chemical information to approach males, where acoustic signals can be used to find breeding ponds, and chemical signals to pick within a chorus (e.g., Rendall et al. 2009; Buxton et al. 2015). The consistency showed by females toward scents obtained from conspecific males (N=3; mean ± SD; weight=4.32 ± 0.67 g; snout-vent length= 31.98 ± 0.53 mm) could subtly suggest that: i) there were no differences between the males’ characteristics or ii) in P. thaul frog, chemical and acoustic signals promote reproduction without involving sexual selection. Future studies must set up experiments that explore all factors influencing chemical behaviour, such as the breeding season and non-breeding season, largest sample size and intra and/or intersexual interactions. In this way, it would be possible to identify the exact role of the chemical compounds in male-female interactions and female preferences.

Conclusions

Our study demonstrates the capability of female Pleurodema thaul to discriminate conspecific male scents from a water control solution, since they remained for longer periods in zones with the conspecific solution compared to the control. Considering this result, we suggest that chemical compounds are involved in the intersexual interactions in this species helping females to find a mate. This study expands our knowledge on chemical communication, which is relevant to understand the factors and mechanism that take part in and modulate reproductive events.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Carlos López and Javier Rivas for assisting us with the experiments. This study was supported by FONDECYT 11140752 assigned to N. A. Velásquez, and Iniciativa Cientifica Milenio, Biomedical Neuroscience Institute, ICM P09-015-F assigned to J. L. Valdés. We also thank Alejandra Fabres who helped with comments on the final version of this manuscript.

References

  • Asay MJ, Harowicz PG, Su L (2005) Chemically mediated mate recognition in the tailed frog (Ascaphus truei). In: Mason RT, LeMaster MP, Müller-Schwarze D (Eds) Chemical Signals in Vertebrates. Springer, Boston, 230–257. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-387-25160-X_4
  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (2011) Principles of Animal Communication. Massachusetts, Sinauer, 38 pp.
  • Brunetti AE, Lyra ML, Melo WG, Andrade LE, Palacios-Rodríguez P, Prado BM, Lopes NP (2019) Symbiotic skin bacteria as a source for sex-specific scents in frogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 116: 2124–2129. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1806834116
  • Buxton VL, Ward MP, Sperry JH (2015) Use of chorus sounds for location of breeding habitat in 2 species of anuran amphibians. Behavioral Ecology 26: 1111–1118. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv059
  • Byrne PG, Keogh JS (2007) Terrestrial toadlets use chemosignals to recognize conspecifics, locate mates and strategically adjust calling behaviour. Animal Behaviour 7: 1155–1162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.033
  • Chivers DP, Ferrari MC (2013) Tadpole antipredator responses change over time: what is the role of learning and generalization? Behavioral Ecology 24: 1114–1121. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/art038
  • Chu J, Wilczynski W (2001) Social influences on androgen levels in the southern leopard frog, Rana sphenocephala. General and Comparative Endocrinology 12: 66–73. https://doi.org/10.1006/gcen.2000.7563
  • Dusenbery DB (1992) Sensory Ecology. W.H. Freeman and Company, New York, 132 pp.
  • Graham BM, O’Hearn DJ, MacAllister IE, Sperry JH (2020) Behavioral Responses by Adult Northern Leopard Frogs to Conspecific Chemical Cues. Journal of Herpetology 54: 168–173. https://doi.org/10.1670/19-029
  • Guillaume O (2000) Role of chemical communication and behavioural interactions among conspecifics in the choice of shelters by the cave-dwelling salamander Proteus anguinus (Caudata, Proteidae). Canadian Journal of Zoology 78: 167–173. https://doi.org/10.1139/z99-198
  • Hepper PG, Waldman B (1992) Embryonic olfactory learning in frogs. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B 44: 179–197.
  • Houck LD, Palmer CA, Watts RA, Arnold SJ, Feldhoff PW, Feldhoff RC (2007) A new vertebrate courtship pheromone, PMF, affects female receptivity in a terrestrial salamander. Animal Behaviour 73: 315–320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.008
  • Houck LD, Watts RA, Arnold SJ, Bowen KE, Kiemnec KM, Godwin HA, Feldhoff PW, Feldhoff RC (2008) A recombinant courtship pheromone affects sexual receptivity in a plethodontid salamander. Chemical Senses 33: 623–631. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjn027
  • Kam Y-C, Yang H-W (2002) Female-offspring communication in a Taiwanese tree frog, Chirixalus eiffingeri (Anura: Rhacophoridae). Animal Behaviour 64: 881–886.
  • Kelley DB (1982) Female sex behaviors in the South African clawed Frog, Xenopus laevis: Gonadotropin-Releasing, Gonadotropic, and steroid hormones. Hormones and Behavior 174: 158–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/0018-506X(82)90016-2
  • Kikuyama S, Toyoda F, Ohmiya Y, Matsuda K, Tanaka S, Hayashi H (1995) Sodefrin: a female-attracting peptide pheromone in newt cloacal glands. Science 226: 1643–1644. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7886452
  • King JD, Rollins-Smith LA, Nielsen PF, John A, Conlon JM (2005) Characterization of a peptide from skin secretions of male specimens of the frog, Leptodactylus fallax that stimulates aggression in male frogs. Peptides 26: 597–601. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peptides.2004.11.004
  • Kiseleva EI (1996) The chemical interaction of tadpoles of the common frog Rana temporaria L. with conspecific and heterospecific anuran tadpoles. Zhurnal Obshchei Biologii 57: 740–746.
  • Lynch KS, Wilczynski W (2008) Reproductive hormones modify reception of species-typical communication signals in a female anuran. Brain Behavior Evolution 71: 143–150. https://doi.org/10.1159/000111460
  • Norris DO, Lopez KH (2011) Hormones and Reproduction of Vertebrates. Amphibian Academic Press; San Diego, 159 pp.
  • Partan SR (2004) Multisensory animal communication. In: Calvert GA, Spence C, Stein BE (Eds) The Handbook of Multisensory Processes. MIT Press, Cambridge, 225–240.
  • Pearl CA, Cervantes M, Chan M, Ho U, Shoji R, Thomas EO (2000) Evidence for a mate-attracting chemosignal in the dwarf African clawed frog Hymenochirus. Hormones and Behavior 38: 67–74. https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.2000.1609
  • Pearl CA, Adams MJ, Schuytema GS, Nebeker AV (2003) Behavioral responses of anuran larvae to chemical cues of native and introduced predators in the Pacific Northwestern United States. Journal of Herpetology 37: 572–577. https://doi.org/10.1670/134-02N
  • Penna M, Veloso A (1990) Vocal diversity in frogs of the South American temperate forest. Journal of Herpetology 24: 23–33. https://doi.org/10.2307/1564285
  • Penna M, Solís R (1998) Frog call intensities and sound propagation in the South American temperate forest region. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 42: 371–381. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650050452
  • Penna M, Velásquez N, Solís R (2008) Correspondence between evoked vocal responses and auditory thresholds in Pleurodema thaul (Amphibia; Leptodactylidae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A 194: 361–371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-007-0312-0
  • Pueta M, Perotti MG (2016) Anuran tadpoles learn to recognize injury cues from members of the same prey guild. Animal Cognition 19: 745–751.
  • Pueta M, Cruz FB, Perotti MG (2016) Feeding regime and food availability determine behavioural decisions under predation risk in Pleurodema thaul (Anura: Leiuperidae) tadpoles. Herpetological Journal 26: 61–64. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-0971-8
  • Roberts JA, Taylor PW, Uetz GW (2003) Kinship and food availability influence cannibalism tendency in early-instar wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 54: 416–422. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-0646-8
  • Rollinson N, Hackett D (2015) Experimental evaluation of agonistic behaviour, chemical communication, spacing, and intersexual associations of the Eastern Red-backed Salamander (Plethodon cinereus) near its northern range limit. Canadian Journal of Zoology 93: 773–781. https://doi.org/10.1139/cjz-2015-0050
  • Schulte LM, Rössler DC (2013) Do poison frogs recognize chemical cues of the other sex or do they react to cues of stressed conspecifics? Behavioural Processes 100: 32–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2013.07.016
  • Still MB, Lea AM, Hofmann HA, Ryan MJ (2019) Multimodal stimuli regulate reproductive behavior and physiology in male túngara frogs. Hormones and Behavior 115: e104546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.06.010
  • Van Bocxlaer I, Maex M, Treer D, Janssenswillen S, Janssens R, Vandebergh W, Proost P. Bossuyt F (2016) Beyond sodefrin: evidence for a multi-component pheromone system in the model newt Cynops pyrrhogaster (Salamandridae). Scientific Reports 6: 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep21880
  • Velásquez NA, Valdés JL, Vásquez RA, Penna M (2015) Lack of phonotactic preferences of female frogs and its consequences for signal evolution. Behavioural Processes 118: 76–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.06.001
  • Wabnitz PA, Bowie JH, Tyler MJ, Wallace JC, Smith BP (1999) Animal behaviour: aquatic sex pheromone from a male tree frog. Nature 401: e444. https://doi.org/10.1038/46724
  • Wabnitz PA, Bowie JH, Tyler MJ, Wallace JC, Smith BP (2000) Differences in the skin peptides of the male and female Australian tree frog Litoria splendida. European Journal of Biochemistry 267: 269–275. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1432-1327.2000.01010.x
  • Watts RA, Palmer CA, Feldhoff RC, Feldhoff PW, Houck LD, Jones AG, Pfrender ME, Rollmann SM, Arnold SJ (2004) Stabilizing selection on behavior and morphology masks positive selection on the signal in a salamander pheromone signalling complex. Molecular Biology and Evolution 21: 1032–1041. https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msh093
  • Wyatt TD (2010) Pheromones and signature mixture: defining species-wide signal and variable cues for identity in both invertebrates and vertebrate. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 196: 685–700. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-010-0564-y
  • Yamamoto K, Kawai Y, Hayashi T, Ohe Y, Hayashi H, Toyoda F, Kawahara G, Iwata T, Kikuyama S (2000) Silefrin, a sodefrin like pheromone in the abdominal gland of the sword tailed newt, Cynops ensicauda. FEBS Letters 472: 267–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0014-5793(00)01455-1
login to comment